Improving language development: read, play, discuss

shutterstock_1077765305

To acquire language, you must first be exposed to language. Simple right? Or maybe not. Sadly, increasing numbers of children are arriving at primary school with insufficient language skills (Bercow, 2008; Lindsay et al., 2010). Insufficient language development can lead to poor narrative, misunderstanding and children being unable to fulfil their potential at school. So, what can we do?

Learning language is predominantly an interactive experience between the learner and a more experienced adult or peer (Bruner, 1981). The most common active techniques for encouraging language development are:

  • shared storybook reading
  • role-playing or story re-telling through play.

Previous research has suggested that for better language learning, activities should be transactional and bidirectional rather than just interactional (Sameroff, 2009), whereby the child both learns and communicates meanings and intentions. Yet interestingly, the role of the adult in the above two common techniques is often passive or one of an assessor.

Whilst transactional activities in the early years has been endorsed by the European Union (2014), there appears to be a lack of support as to how to embed these activities into everyday practice without becoming a bystander or assessor. The difficulty comes when trying to find an activity or task that strikes the perfect balance between guiding the direction of the reading or play to adhere to lesson or session requirements, whilst also allowing every child to independently explore newly experienced material (Dickinson and Neelands, 2006).

The current study attempted to find that balance. Lake and Evangelou (2019) designed and delivered an intervention (Let’s Talk) to try and support young children’s oral development and vocabulary. Crucially, the role of the adult during these sessions was to actively support and encourage language development so the sessions facilitated interaction with the adult as well as between peers and with the materials.

The Let's Talk intervention was designed to find a balance between adult involvement and children's independent exploration of new material.

The Let’s Talk intervention was designed to find a balance between adult involvement and children’s independent exploration of new material.

Methods

The study recruited 94 children aged 37-55 months. 52 were placed in the intervention group, and 42 were placed in the no-intervention group (whose data was used for comparison purposes). The splitting of children randomly into two comparison groups makes this study a randomised controlled trial (RCT). English was the first language for 85% of children.

The intervention: Let’s talk

Groups of 3-5 children participated in 20-25 minute sessions twice a week for 10 weeks.

The intervention had two aspects:

  • A group-shared storybook reading session followed by dialogic discussion with a puppet asking questions. A different storybook was read and used each week. The books were chosen for their repetitive text, engaging illustrations, humour and length (see below for list of books).
  • A few days later children took part in a pretend-play. The adult took on a role from the book and encouraged the children to take on other roles to act out the story. The adult presented the children with problems from the story and the children set-up the play area and decided how props could be used to solve those problems. After the pretend-play session, the adult encouraged discussion around the play session.

The puppet turned the pages of the storybook, modelled listening and asking questions, acted as the audience for the children to retell the story and encouraged the use of new vocabulary in sentences.

No-intervention group

No-intervention (comparison) groups (3-5 children) were carried out twice a week for 10 weeks for 20-25 minutes. Children completed numeracy activities and games.

Measures

All children completed the following tests pre- and post-intervention:

  • Receptive vocabulary (the British Picture Vocabulary Scales II – Naming Vocabulary: children are shown a series of pictures and asked to say what each one is)
  • Productive vocabulary (a naming vocabulary test and target word assessment – common but currently novel nouns were taken from the storybooks and used and measured throughout the intervention)
  • Narrative assessments (the mean length of utterance: MLU)
  • Dimensional change card sort to measure executive function ((cognitive control of behaviour): children must sort a set of cards by one categorisation e.g., by colour and then switch when requested, to sort the cards by another categorisation e.g., by shape).

Eighty-three percent of parents completed a family background questionnaire.

Results

The researchers looked at the outcome data whilst controlling for pre-test data to identify the exact influence of the intervention. They also found that there were no significant differences between the two groups of children at baseline showing that the initial randomisation was successful.

There were 5 main findings:

  • Children who took part in the intervention were much improved on narrative ability compared to the no-intervention group
  • Executive function did not predict narrative ability
  • Being able to grasp the story grammar was not affected by group
  • Children who took part in the intervention showed significantly improved receptive vocabulary compared to the no-intervention group
  • Children who took part in the intervention also showed an overall increase in productive vocabulary. This improvement was significant for the taught target vocabulary.
The Let's Talk intervention improved children's receptive and productive vocabulary and narrative ability independent of executive function.

The Let’s Talk intervention improved children’s receptive and productive vocabulary and narrative ability independent of executive function.

Conclusions

The authors found these transactional, adult-immersed group storybook readings with dialogic discussions, and pretend play sessions to be beneficial for children’s vocabulary development (receptive and productive). Children’s narrative ability was enhanced with this combined activity, and pretend play based on the storybook encouraged and enabled children to acquire and hold new (target) words in their working memory long enough to transfer and store in long-term memory.

Transactional, adult-immersed group activities were beneficial for vocabulary development and enhanced narrative ability.

Transactional, adult-immersed group activities were beneficial for vocabulary development and enhanced narrative ability.

Strengths and limitations

This study provides support for existing theories whilst contributing additional information about the use of adult-supported activities in a pedagogical situation. The design of the intervention has practitioners at the forefront and as such, is very simple to administer and is flexible: the authors have listed specific books and props, but the intervention framework could be applied to others. It is also a very inexpensive intervention but somewhat expensive in terms of time: carrying out 20-25 minutes hands-on time twice a week with 3-5 children at a time for 10 weeks. However, there are also other interventions that require more time and show weaker outcomes. Perhaps, the intervention is only needed for children whose language skills are poor, but this would require additional time on pre-assessments.

The authors conducted an RCT which is considered the gold-standard method for testing the efficacy of an intervention. Random allocation to groups allowed the authors to directly compare the impact of their intervention to ‘practice as usual’, without the influence of mere involvement in the study. However, in this study it was a researcher acting as the adult in the interactions which may not translate directly to a wide range of practitioners with different backgrounds, interaction styles and who did not develop the intervention themselves. Researcher-led interventions are not as ecologically valid as practitioners leading the researchers’ interventions. The next step in this process would be to replicate the findings with practitioners using the intervention. The authors were clear on the procedure and if the the structure for sessions was provided, or a script / set of prompts then that would ensure a more standardised comparison.

15% of children had English as an additional language (EAL). This is low compared to the national average for primary school EAL learners (just over 21% of the primary population in England: The Bell Foundation, 2019). Although, they reported finding no difference between EAL and non-EAL learners in the overall developmental trajectory, it is worth considering whether there might be differences in acquiring new language skills and information between the two groups and perhaps stronger results had the data been considered separately.

The authors used an RCT to test the intervention but what about its real-world applicability?

The authors used an RCT to test the intervention but what about its real-world applicability?

Implications for practice

Expectations of practitioners can be unclear when it comes to play sessions, and often they are left as observers or assessors. The authors conclude that they have created an intervention that “has the potential to overcome some of these challenges as it can be used as a tool for the professional development of early years practitioners in play-based pedagogies”. The intervention is simple to use, has good developmental outcomes and requires minimal training. It can be adapted to the resources available and is easy to embed into a pre-school day.

Books used in the intervention (see original paper for full details / props used):

  1. A squash and a squeeze (Donaldson & Scheffler)
  2. Abby’s chairs (Santucci)
  3. Lost and found (Jeffers)
  4. Please don’t chat to the bus driver (Newton)
  5. Nora: The girl who ate and ate… (Weale)
  6. The giant jam sandwich (Burroway)
  7. The shopping basket (Burningham)
  8. Dogs go shopping (Rennta)
  9. The jolly postman or other people’s letters (Ahlberg & Ahlberg)
  10. Doctor de soto (Steig)
The intervention is simple to use, has good developmental outcomes and requires minimal training. It can be adapted to the resources available and is easy to embed into a pre-school day.

The Let’s Talk intervention is simple to use, has good developmental outcomes and requires minimal training. It can be adapted to the resources available and is easy to embed into a pre-school day.

Conflicts of interest 

None.

Links

Primary paper

Lake, G., & Evangelou, M. (2019). Let’s Talk! An interactive intervention to support children’s language development. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 27(2), 221-240. https://doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2019.1579549

Other references

Bercow, J. (2008). The Bercow Report: A review of services for children and young people (0-19) with speech, language and communication needs. Communication. Nottingham: DCSF.

Bruner, J. (1981). The social context of language acquisition. Language & Communication, 1(2), 155-178. [Abstract]

Dickinson, R., & Neelands, J. (2006). Improve Your Primary School Through Drama. London: David Fulton.

European Commission/EACE/Eurydice/Eurostat. (2014). Key Data on Early Childhood Education and Care. Luxembourg: European Commission/EACE/Eurydice/Eurostat.

Lindsay, G., Dockrell, J., Desforges, M., Law, J., & Peacey, N. (2010). Meeting the needs of children and young people with speech, language and communication difficulties. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders / Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists, 45(4), 448-460. [Abstract]

Sameroff, A. J. (2009). The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

The Bell Foundation (2019). EAL learners in the UK.

Photo credits

Share on Facebook Tweet this on Twitter Share on LinkedIn Share on Google+